Perceived Severity as a Potential Mediator in Estimation of Disease Frequencies Felicia Trembath
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.53555/nnssh.v4i1.141Keywords:
Perceived, Severity, as, Mediator, in, Estimation, Disease, FrequenciesAbstract
A heuristic is a mental strategy for making judgments quickly and efficiently. The recognition heuristic was first described by Goldstein and Gigerenzer in 2002 (Goldstein & Gigerenzer, 2002), and holds that when an individual is asked to choose between two objects and they only recognize one of the two objects, that they will choose the recognized object. This concept has been tested in a variety of situations from asking individuals to choose the city with the largest population size to having them choose who will win a rugby match. In most instances individuals adhere to the recognition heuristic about 90% of the time (Goldstein & Gigerenzer, 2002). That is to say in situations where an individual recognizes only one of the choices presented, 90% of the time they will select the recognized item. Individuals are more likely to select the recognized item when under a time constraint to make a decision (Pachur & Hertwig, 2006). However, a strange phenomenon has been demonstrated when individuals are asked to estimate frequencies of diseases. When participants are asked to estimate rates of disease frequencies, the adherence rate to the recognition heuristic drops to 62% (Pachur & Hertwig, 2006). According to the principle of the recognition heuristic, it is applied when one item is recognized and the other is not. However, when asked to estimate frequencies of diseases, 38% of the time participants recognize only one of the diseases, yet they chose not to use the recognition heuristic. The lower adherence rate in the case of estimation of disease frequencies is a puzzling phenomenon and suggests that participants may be considering other information instead of, or in addition to recognition. Perhaps in estimations of city populations in foreign countries, the only information that is known is that the individual has heard the name of one of the cities in the media, thus the recognition heuristic is followed 90% of the time (Goldstein & Gigerenzer, 2002). On the other hand, individuals have ideas and opinions about diseases. In fact, they or a close member of their social network may have even been afflicted by the disease in question. This situation thus poses one where an individual has pre-existing notions about diseases and may rely upon these notions rather than recognition alone in formulating estimates of disease frequencies. In the inception of the recognition heuristic, Goldstein and Gigerenzer (2002) postulated that “no other information about the unrecognized object is searched for and therefore, no other information can reverse the choice determined by recognition (p. 82).” Yet the discrepancy in adherence to the recognition heuristic in the case of estimating disease frequencies calls into question whether in some situations individuals are in fact searching for and considering other information (Bröder & Eichler, 2006; Oppenheimer, 2003; Pachur & Hertwig, 2006). It stands to reason that if no information is known about an object, that no information about that object can be considered when making an estimation of frequency. However, when testing the recognition heuristic, researchers merely measure whether or not a person selects the recognized object, any other information that an individual knows or perceives about the two choices remains unmeasured. Thus there is an unidentified amount of information known about the recognized choice, which could be considered by the individual making a selection. In the case of estimation of disease frequencies, the unidentified knowledge is likely the individual’s perceived severity of that disease. This article proposes that when estimating disease frequencies the application of the recognition heuristic is mediated by perceived disease severity, and further describes the plausible mechanism by which that meditation may occur.
References
Becker, M. H. (1974). The health belief model and personal health behavior. Thorofare, NJ: Slack.
Bröder, A., & Eichler, A. (2006). The use of recognition information and additional cues in inferences from memory. Acta psychologica, 121(3), 275-284. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.07.001
DiMatteo, M. R., Haskard, K. B., & Williams, S. L. (2007). Health beliefs, disease severity, and patient adherence: a meta-analysis. Medical care, 45(6), 521. doi:10.1097/MLR.0b013e318032937e
Goldstein, D. G., & Gigerenzer, G. (2002). Models of ecological rationality: The recognition heuristic. Psychological Review, 109(1), 75-90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.109.1.75
Knaus, W. A., Draper, E. A., Wagner, D. P., & Zimmerman, J. E. (1985). APACHE II: a severity of disease classification system. Critical care medicine, 13(10), 818.
Oppenheimer, D. M. (2003). Not so fast!(and not so frugal!): Rethinking the recognition heuristic. Cognition, 90(1), B1-B9. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00141-0
Pachur, T., & Hertwig, R. (2006). On the psychology of the recognition heuristic: Retrieval primacy as a key determinant of its use. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(5), 983-1002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.983
Pennington, H. (2004). Why can’t doctors be more scientific. London Review of Books, 26(13), 28-29.
Rosenstock, I. M. (1974). Historical origins of the health belief model. Health education monographs, 2(4),
-335.
Published
Issue
Section
License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
Terms & Condition
Submission -
Author can submit the manuscript through our online submission process or email us at the designated email id in contact details.
The other mode of submission not accepted than online and email.
Before submission please read the submission guidelines.
NN Publication accepts only article submitted in pdf/doc/docx/rtf file format. Another format except given file formats will no be considered .
Author will be responsible for the error mistakes in the submission files. The minor changes can be done without any cost after publication. But for major changes NN Publication may charges you the editing charges.
Publication (Online) -
The online publication is scheduled on last date of every month, but it can be delayed by 24 to 48 hours due to editorial process if huge number of articles comes to publish in single issue.
Automatic notificatation email will be sent to the all users on publication of an issue, so its author’s duty to check their email inbox or SPAM folder to get this notification.
After publication of article author can not withdraw their article.
If editor’s found any issue after publication of article then the NN Publication have the authority to remove the article from online website.
No refund will be provided after online publication of article.
Publication (Print) -
The print copy publication are sent as per the author’s request after 2 weeks of online publication of that issue.
NN Publication will ship the article by India Post and provide the consignment number on dispatch of print copy.
NN Publication follows all the guidelines of delivery provided by IndiaPost and hence not responsible for delay in delivery due to any kind of reasons.
Refund of hard copy will not be provided after dispatch or print of the journal.
NN Publication will be responsible for raise a complain if there is any issue occurs in delivery, but still will not be responsible for providing the refund.
NN Publication will be responsible to resend the print copy only and only if the print copy is lost or print copy is damaged in delivery / or there is delay more than 6 months.
According to India Post the delivery should be completed with in 1-3 weeks after dispatch of articles.
Privacy Policy-
NN Publicationl uses the email ids of authors and editors and readers for sending editorial or publication notification only, we do not reveal or sell the email ids to any other website or company.